The concept of phoneme has been studied and explained in three different schools of phonetics. Firstly, according to a school, the phoneme represents a physical phonetic reality. According to David Jones (1931), a phoneme is a family of sounds in a given language and consists of an important sound of the language together with other related sounds. It takes place in particular sound sequences. Gleason (1955) defines the phoneme as a class of sounds that are phonetically similar and show certain characteristic patterns of distribution in the language or dialect under consideration. Under this view, the phoneme is seen as a convenient label for several phonetic units. For example, /p/ may stand for [p], [p:], [ph], etc.
Secondly, in the Prague school, phoneme is defined in
purely phonological terms. According to Trubetzkoy (1939), the phoneme is the
sum of the phonologically relevant properties of a sound. For him, phonemes are
defined in terms of oppositions in a phonological system. Thus, a phoneme is a
minimal unit that can function to distinguish meanings. It is not a sound or
even a group of sounds, but rather an abstraction, a theoretical construct on
the phonological level.
Thirdly, the phoneme is defined as a mental reality.
It is considered to be the outcome of the intention of the speaker, or the
impression of the hearer, or both (Twaddell 1935).
In sum, the phoneme theory considers the different
definitions of the phoneme and attempts to describe the distinctive sounds of a
particular language and the relations existing between sounds.
Rules for Establishing Phonemes:
To establish the phonemes of a particular language,
different phoneticians and phonologists have invested their endeavors and hence
postulated varied principles and rules. The goal is to determine which sounds
function as distinctive units that can change meaning. Here are the key rules
and methods:
1.
Minimal Pair Test:
A minimal pair is a pair of words that
differ by only one sound in the same position and have different meanings. The purpose
of this test is to determine if two sounds create a minimal pair; they are
considered separate phonemes. For example, pat /pæt/ vs. bat /bæt/---- here /p/
and /b/ are different phonemes in English.
Two sounds are in contrastive distribution
if they can occur in the same environment and change meaning. If sounds
contrast, they are separate phonemes. For example, /t/ and /d/ in ten and den.
Sounds occur in different environments and
never contrast; they are allophones of the same phoneme. For example, [ph]
(aspirated) in pin vs. [p] (unaspirated) in spin are allophones of /p/ in
English.
4.
Phonetic Similarity:
allophones of the same phoneme must be
phonetically similar. For example, [t] and [th] are similar in articulation (both
are voiceless alveolar stops).
5.
Native Speaker Intuition:
Native speakers usually don’t perceive
allophones as different sounds, but they perceive different phonemes clearly.
For example, English speakers don’t notice [ph] vs. [p], but they notice /p/ vs
/b/.
6.
Free variation (optional consideration):
Sometimes, two sounds occur in the same
environment but do not change meaning and are used interchangeably. These
sounds are in free variation and may he treated as allophones.
Summary:
Criterion |
Outcome |
Minimal pair |
Separate phonemes |
Contrastive distribution |
Separate phonemes |
Complementary distribution |
Allophones of the same phoneme |
Phonetic similarity |
Supports allophone classification |
Native Speaker Intuition |
Supports phonemic distinction |
No comments:
Post a Comment